Annals of Indian Academy of Neurology
  Users Online: 3450 Home | About the Journal | InstructionsCurrent Issue | Back IssuesLogin      Print this page Email this page  Small font size Default font size Increase font size

Table of Contents
REVIEW ARTICLE
Year : 2014  |  Volume : 17  |  Issue : 4  |  Page : 374-382
 

Neuropsychological deficits in temporal lobe epilepsy: A comprehensive review


1 Department of Emergency, Yantai Yuhuangding Hospital, Yantai 264000, Shandong Province, China
2 Operating RoomYantaishan Hospital, Yantai 264000, Shandong Province, China
3 Department of Clinical Psychology, Ranchi Institute of Neuropsychiatry and Allied Sciences, Kanke, Ranchi, Jharkhand, India
4 Department of Physiology, M. S. Ramaiah Medical College, Mathikere, Bengaluru, Karnataka, India
5 Department of Psychiatry, Post Graduate Institute of Medical Education and Research and Dr. Ram Manohar Lohia Hospital, Formerly Willingdon Hospital, New Delhi, India

Date of Submission17-Mar-2014
Date of Decision09-May-2013
Date of Acceptance24-Mar-2014
Date of Web Publication5-Nov-2014

Correspondence Address:
Priyanka Rastogi
Department of Clinical Psychology, Ranchi Institute of Neuropsychiatry and Allied Sciences, Kanke, Ranchi
India
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/0972-2327.144003

Rights and Permissions

 

   Abstract 

Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is the most prevalent form of complex partial seizures with temporal lobe origin of electrical abnormality. Studies have shown that recurrent seizures affect all aspects of cognitive functioning, including memory, language, praxis, executive functions, and social judgment, among several others. In this article, we will review these cognitive impairments along with their neuropathological correlates in a comprehensive manner. We will see that neuropsychological deficits are prevalent in TLE. Much of the effort has been laid on memory due to the notion that temporal lobe brain structures involved in TLE play a central role in consolidating information into memory. It seems that damage to the mesial structure of the temporal lobe, particularly the amygdale and hippocampus, has the main role in these memory difficulties and the neurobiological plausibility of the role of the temporal lobe in different aspects of memory. Here, we will cover the sub-domains of working memory and episodic memory deficits. This is we will further proceed to evaluate the evidences of executive function deficits in TLE and will see that set-shifting among other EFs is specifically affected in TLE as is social cognition. Finally, critical components of language related deficits are also found in the form of word-finding difficulties. To conclude, TLE affects several of cognitive function domains, but the etiopathogenesis of all these dysfunctions remain elusive. Further well-designed studies are needed for a better understanding of these disorders.


Keywords: Neuropsychological deficits, temporal lobe epilepsy, neurological correlates


How to cite this article:
Zhao F, Kang H, You L, Rastogi P, Venkatesh D, Chandra M. Neuropsychological deficits in temporal lobe epilepsy: A comprehensive review. Ann Indian Acad Neurol 2014;17:374-82

How to cite this URL:
Zhao F, Kang H, You L, Rastogi P, Venkatesh D, Chandra M. Neuropsychological deficits in temporal lobe epilepsy: A comprehensive review. Ann Indian Acad Neurol [serial online] 2014 [cited 2019 Dec 6];17:374-82. Available from: http://www.annalsofian.org/text.asp?2014/17/4/374/144003



   Introduction Top


Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is the most common type of complex partial seizures (CPS). In addition to seizures, this condition also presents with several varied forms of notorious clinical features. A more concerning aspect of TLE is its cognitive sequelae. Recently, several studies have shown that recurrent seizures affect all aspects of cognitive functioning including attention, language, praxis, executive function (intelligence), judgment, insight, and problem solving. [1],[2],[3] The aim of this review is to highlight and elaborate these cognitive impairments in TLE patients in a comprehensive manner as it is impossible to detail all these deficits in a single review. We will thus take an ariel view on the main neuropsychological findings and will try to address only the relevant and landmark studies conducted in this context. For the purpose of this review, we will divide the abnormal cognitive findings into:

  1. Memory impairments
  2. Executive function impairments
  3. Language impairments and
  4. Other cognitive impairments.


Memory impairments

Memory deficits in TLE have been the most studied among all the neuropsychological domains. This seems to be because of the neurobiological implication of the temporal lobes, for memory, and related functions. [4] There are several domains of memory which are affected by TLE. Here are we will address two of these sub-domains of memory, which have been explored the most scientifically. These are the working memory (WM) and episodic memory related deficits.

Working memory deficits

Among all the sub-domains of memory, WM has received much attention as evident by the plethora of studies in this field. Short-term memory is a transient trace of information temporarily stored that requires consolidation by the support of the medial temporal lobe (MTL) to be converted into a more stable status of long-term memory (LTM). WM impairment carries immense clinical importance not only because of the disability that it leads to, but also because of its association with LTM. This association has been highlighted in the recent models of WM in the role of the "episodic buffer" thus linking it to LTM systems [5] [Figure 1]. Thus consequently, LTM will also be affected if STM is impaired in TLE. As we proceed to the next section of autobiographical memory, this association will become clearer from several neuropsychological evidences.
Figure 1: Schematic diagram of Baddeley's working memory model

Click here to view


Several neuropsychological studies provide evidence of WM dysfunction in TLE [Table 1]. [6],[7],[8],[9] The WM impairments affect both visuospatial [6] as well as verbal WM abilities in such patients. [9] The clinical and pathological correlates of these deficits will be dealt in next sub-section.
Table 1: Studies on the effects of TLE on working memory

Click here to view


Recently, dissociation-theories have been proposed between WM and other domains of memory in TLE. [10] On the basis of an experiment involving face-recognition test hypothesized that a double dissociation of short-term and LTM exists in TLE and idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Similar dissociation theories exist for the connections between WM and meta-memory. For example, [11] found intact meta-memory in TLE patients in spite of impairments in WM.

Clinical and pathological correlates of working memory impairments

[Table 2] shows that there are three factors that emerges from our literature review which is associated with impaired WM in TLE patients. The most important one of them seems to be the number seizures/early age of onset. More and more studies are making it evident that the severity of WM impairment in TLE is associated with number of seizures. More recently, Black et al. [7] investigated the impact of the duration of epilepsy and lifetime seizure load on frontal lobe function in 207 TLE patients and later conducted a multivariate regression analyses to find that age at onset was the strongest predictor of WM and executive function so that earlier onset of TLE was predictive of poorer performance on the subtests of WM and executive functions. The second important neuropathological correlate of WM deficits is the side of brain originating the TLE, popularly known as lateralization. In the only study to look specifically at material-specific lateralization of WM in TLE, Wagner et al. [9] studied a group of 96 patients with unilateral MTL damage (24 preoperative and 72 postoperative) on matched verbal and nonverbal supra span tasks. There was a significant interaction between material type and side of pathology although both groups showed reduced span sizes for both material types. The left TLE group made significantly more errors on the verbal span task, but not on the visuospatial task when compared with controls and right TLE patients. These results were considered to argue for material-specific lateralization of WM dysfunction arising from unilateral left MTL damage. [9] On the other hand, the works by found that spatial memory deficits across both working and reference memory conditions were found in patients with a right epileptogenic focus. Düzel et al.[12] induced changes in verbal WM performance in patients with left TLE using transcranial magnetic stimulation over the temporal lobe. The authors argued that the phonological loop, responsible for the short-term storage of verbal information, has a functionally and anatomically multi-modular structure including both frontal and temporal areas.
Table 2: Three important factors related with severity of impaired working memory in TLE patients

Click here to view


Finally, the third important neuropathological correlate for WM decline in TLE patients seems to be hippocampal sclerosis (HS). Earlier, Abrahams et al. [6] they found that patients with right hippocampal damage had impairments on spatial WM task, and that hippocampal and parahippocampal gyri volume negatively correlated with the number of spatial memory errors. In a recent study by Campo et al., [13] it was shown that HS alters the functional connectivity needed for maintaining and executing the WM function. Especially, MTL damage (sclerosis) weakened backward connections from left MTL to left inferior temporal cortex, which was accompanied by strengthening of bidirectional connections between inferior frontal cortex and MTL in the contra-lesional hemisphere.

Autobiographical memory deficits

In recent years, a subset of patients with TLE has been identified and subsequently explored who display normal, or even above normal performance at standard delays of recall (i.e., ∼30 min), but impaired performance over longer periods of retention (i.e., days or weeks), suggesting an alteration of memory consolidation mechanisms. [23],[24],[25] This memory loss has been extensively studied in transient epileptic amnesia, a subtype of TLE where memory deficit is the core symptom of ictal and interictal manifestations. [25],[26],[27],[28],[29],[30] In this syndrome, patients typically present with transient episodes of amnesia and most of them also disclose accelerated forgetting rate along with an isolated autobiographical memory deficit. [25],[28],[29],[30] In such patients, autobiographical memory deficits are due to selective long-term consolidation deficits for autobiographical memories as evidenced by accelerated rate of forgetting.

A number of recent group and case studies [31],[32] provide ample evidence indicating that TEA patients have deficits for autobiographical event memory. For instance, Butler et al., [29] who recruited 50 patients diagnosed with TEA from across the UK as part of the TIME project, found that 70% of the patients complained of autobiographical memory deficits. In another report, Kopelman et al. [33] found that the autobiographical memory impairments extended back several decades. More recently, Milton et al. [30] tested 14 patients and 11 age and intelligent quotient matched controls using the autobiographical interview, which is currently the most sensitive test of autobiographical memory performance. This revealed autobiographical memory deficits across the entire lifespan. In addition, these impairments were present for all the different types of contextual information that were examined (event, time, place, perceptual, and thought/emotion details).

If the basic defect behind these LTM impairments is in storage or retrieval still remains unclear. Evidence from patient R.G., [34] mentioned earlier is consistent with the notion that these autobiographical memory deficits in TEA reflect a storage problem. Whereas, R.G.'s performance did not benefit from salient verbal and visual cues, a recent case study showed that a patient with TEA recovered previously lost, personally significant memories following episodes of déjà vu [35] raising the possibility that these deficits may, at least partially, be due to a retrieval deficit. One explanation often given for the autobiographical memory deficits in TEA is that clinical or subclinical activity propagates from the MTL to neocortical regions of the autobiographical memory network and this disrupts the memory trace (e.g., [25],[30] ).

Tramoni et al. [36] took a step ahead in understanding the episodic memory deficits in TLE patients by specifically evaluating the cognitive processes for consolidation of LTM. They specifically investigated long-term consolidation of memory in a group of five adult-onset pharmacosensitive patients with TLE, exhibiting severe episodic memory complaints despite normal performance at standardized memory assessment in a two-step experiment. In the first step of the experiment, the magnitude of autobiographical memory loss was evaluated using retrograde personal memory tasks based on verbal and visual cues. In both conditions, results showed an unusual U-shaped pattern of personal memory impairment, which was suggestive of long-term impairments in consolidation of personal episodes, adequately consolidated over "short-term" delays but gradually forgotten thereafter. To explore further, they conducted the second step, where patients were specifically investigated for short and long-term consolidation of contextually-bound experiences (episodic memory) and context-free information (semantic knowledge and single-items). In the short-term (1 h), performance of both contextually-free and contextually-bound memory tasks was intact. After a 6-week delay, however, contextually-bound memory performance was impaired while contextually-free memory performance remained preserved. This effect was independent of task difficulty and the modality of retrieval (recall and recognition). For pathological understanding of this finding, neuroimaging was also done for these patients who revealed the presence of mild metabolic changes within MTL structures. Authors concluded that mild MTL dysfunction can impede the building and stabilization of episodic memories, but leaves long-term semantic and single-items mnemonic traces intact.

Executive function impairments

Interestingly, although most studies have demonstrated the impairment of executive function deficits in TLE [Table 3], the explanations that they have hypothesized for this impairment have differed to a wide extent. These variations are rooted in the fact that there is no clarity on the specific role of temporal lobe in the context of executive functions. Hermann et al. [37] for example found that 44% of patients exhibited a clinically relevant executive dysfunction on Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST). The authors suggested that deficits in executive function may be associated with propagation of temporal lobe seizure activity to relevant areas of executive skill. Another neuropathological explanation was provided by Strauss et al. [38] who assessed 77 TLE patients with the WCST and found that set shifting ability was most impaired by the presence of left temporal lobe dysfunction, but only if damage occurred before the age of 1 year. Deficits in right TLE set shifting ability were less severe, but occurred independent to the age of onset. In contrast to these neurological perspectives there have also been some cognitive explanations for these deficits. Corcoran and Upton [39] found that HS, a pathological hallmark of mesial TLE (MTLE), compromised performance on the modified WCST (MWCST) such that HS patients completed fewer categories and made more preservative errors compared to frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE) and nonHS TLE groups. Authors argued that their results provide evidence for a dissociation between executive subsystems [39] as discussed before. A very different argument was given by Giovagnoli [40] who investigated the contribution of the hippocampus to performance on the MWCST. The performance of TLE patients with left HS was significantly impaired. There was also a trend for left TLE patients without HS to perform poorly. The authors argued that HS patients were compromised in their ability to form associations and register new information, two processes that are critical for the successful completion of the task. Several other studies have also found performance on the WCST to be compromised in TLE [2],[41],[42],[43],[44] and specifically those with HS. [40],[45],[46] Their explanations have varied from neurobiological to cognitive perspectives as mentioned above.
Table 3: Studies of executive function in TLE

Click here to view


There has been limited research using other measures of executive functions in TLE. These assessments have their own merits in assessing EFs. In fact, WCST may be inadequate on some fronts of EF assessments. For example, Rzezak et al. [47] evaluated 35 children with TLE and 25 healthy controls with the WCST and with a more comprehensive battery. Among the children with TLE, 77.14% showed impairment on the WCST. On other tests (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Digit Forward, Matching Familiar Figures Test, Trail Making Test, Word Fluency, Finger Windows, and Number-Letter Memory), impairment was demonstrated in 94.29%. The authors concluded that the WCST is a good paradigm to measure executive impairment in children with TLE; however, it may be not enough. Evaluation performed only with the WCST not only underestimated the number of patients with erectile dysfunction (ED), but also missed relevant information regarding the type of ED.

Labudda et al. [48] assessed feedback based decision-making in 20 TLE patients using the Iowa gambling task. [49] The test requires subjects to select a card (typically with monetary value) from one of four decks, two decks provide short-term gain and long-term loss (disadvantageous decision), and two provide short-term loss but long-term gain (advantageous decision). Subjects are assessed on their ability to utilize the immediate feedback from each deck in order to make greater advantageous than disadvantageous decisions. Their results depicted that compared with controls, TLE patients were significantly impaired in their decision-making, and those with a preference for disadvantageous decisions performed less well on other tests of executive functions as well.

While these studies provide convincing evidence for executive dysfunction in TLE, there have also been some contradictory findings. McDonald et al. [50] administered the Trail Making Test, a measure of mental flexibility, to FLE patients, TLE patients, and healthy controls. FLE patients showed significant impairment in both speed and accuracy compared to TLE and controls in the more cognitively demanding set switching condition. Fewer studies have taken up the task of exploring the clinic-pathological correlates of these EF impairments and the details of all of them are out of scope here. However, two of these studies warrant special mention here. Findings of the retrospective study by Thompson and Duncan [51] suggested that as duration of epilepsy and the number of CPSs increase, executive functions decline. Similarly, [7] found through their multivariate analysis that earlier age at seizure onset, longer duration, and higher lifetime seizure frequency affect cognitive functioning in both TLE.

Language impairments in temporal lobe epilepsy

In addition to the cognitive domains as mentioned above, language impairments have been consistently observed in TLE. Word finding difficulties have been the most studied among them. Multiple studies have reported that up to 40% of patients with TLE and a speech dominant focus have a clinically significant deficit in naming abilities. [59],[60] Word finding difficulties that interfere with daily life activities are frequently reported by epileptic patients whose seizures originate in the language dominant cerebral hemisphere. [61] Recently, in a study of Spanish speakers with pharmacoresistant TLE, Lomlomdjian et al. [62] observed that almost one-third of patients reported frequent and severe word finding problems during spontaneous speech. In naming tests, the patients exhibited delayed time durations for finding words. Even if the target word was identified and semantically activated, there was a difficulty with lexical access, which improved when a phonetic cue was given. Left TLE patients derived a lower benefit from phonetic cues in accessing words, even when the word was known and recognized semantically. Word finding abnormalities in TLE were originally investigated with the picture naming tasks. Hamberger et al. however used oral definitions instead of pictures for eliciting patients responses. The definition task has proved to be more sensitive than the picture task when it comes to detecting preoperational TLE language deficits. [59],[63],[64] For example, hamburger and Tamny used both auditory and visual naming tasks in TLE patients. They found that left TLE group obtained significantly lower scores than other groups on auditory naming, whereas their performance on visual naming was indistinguishable from that of right TLE patients and normal. Furthermore, whereas cut-off scores on the auditory naming task predicted seizure focus laterality in 85% of patients, performance on the visual naming task predicted laterality in only 60% of patients.

Neurobiology of naming defects in temporal lobe epilepsy

It is well-established that naming function is mediated by the perisylvian cortex in the language dominant hemisphere. Healthy speakers producing words during functional imaging show widespread activation of left perisylvian and extrasylvian cortex. During picture naming, distribution of activity in the left anterior, inferior and posterior middle/superior temporal cortex, posterior inferior frontal and inferior parietal cortex has been shown. [65],[66],[67],[68],[69],[70],[71] More recently, evidence has been accumulating from cortical stimulation studies [72] as well as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies that hippocampus is directly involved in naming functions. These findings support the notion that separate components of the common network supporting naming are required differentially for distinct cognitive processes [69],[73]

Jensen et al. [74] examined the lexical and semantic processing in left-sided TLE patients by comparing behavioral and neuroimaging data associated with words and nonwords (lexicality) or with concrete and abstract words (concreteness). In addition, brain activation was studied using fMRI. Although the control group showed greater activation associated with word stimuli than with nonword stimuli in a bilateral language network, TLE groups showed greater activation for nonword stimuli than word stimuli. The TLE groups also exhibited differential activation patterns during the processing of abstract and concrete words compared to controls. For abstract words, in particular, the patient having HS group showed activation of frontal areas typically associated with executive functions, whereas the nonlesional patients (NL group) showed activation of more posterior semantic processing regions.

Understanding this language supporting network is important in understanding and integrating various theories of language impairments in TLE. In TLE patients difficulties in word retrieval, most clearly seen in the definition task, are associated with more anterior temporal sites. This could apparently be due to fundamental specifics versus reorganization of the language system in pharmacoresistant epilepsy. [72],[75] Alternatively, it could point to the need for characterizing naming performance and naming failure in TLE in a much more detailed manner, for example using the processing model. A central aspect of such a model is the distinction between concept identification and word form retrieval. The importance of this distinction was previously noted by Damasio et al., [73] who distinguished tip-of-the-tongue states from correct naming scores. Drane et al. (2008) also distinguished recognition from naming processes in patients with anterior temporal lobectomy. In the latter study, patients with nondominant TLE exhibited an identification deficit, whereas patients with dominant lobe resection mostly suffered from word retrieval deficits. [77] Other studies showed that naming difficulties in TLE were more likely to be due to lexical retrieval problems associated with the temporal lobe network. [78]

The neural basis of these language deficits in left TLE patients has also been investigated using electricocortical stimulation mapping. For example, stimulation of brain areas during picture naming has been the task of choice for identifying language cortex based on positive naming sites during presurgical mapping (Ojemann et al., 1989). Within the temporal lobe, Hamberger et al. have shown that definition naming sites (i.e., sites at which stimulation impairs auditory but not visual naming) are generally located anterior to visual naming sites or so-called "dual" sites (i.e., sites at which stimulation disrupts both auditory and visual naming). [76],[79] Hara et al. [80] used a phonetic oddball paradigm in patients with TLE to elicit the mismatch negativity (MMN) response at frontocentral sites and the mismatch positivity (MMP) response at mastoid sites. The MMN in 26 patients were compared with that of 26 age- and gender-matched healthy control participants. Electroencephalography responses were recorded during the presentation of speech sounds: The vowels "a" and "o" in alternation. Average waveforms were obtained for standard and deviant trials. They found that the MMP responses at bilateral mastoid sites were reduced, whereas the MMN response at frontocentral sites did not change significantly. These results support the view that the MMN is generated by separable sources in the frontal and temporal lobes and that these sources are differentially affected by TLE.

Laterality and speech in temporal lobe epilepsy patients

Laterality in TLE patients seems to contribute to the language related abnormalities. Recently, fMRI has been frequently used for assessing laterality related issues in language impairments in TLE especially because of its noninvasive nature and high reproducibility for localizing brain parts involved in processing language function. [81],[82] fMRI studies using simple phonemic verbal fluency paradigms have reliably lateralized language dominance in healthy controls and TLE patients by showing left frontal lobe activation corresponding to Broca's area, and less prominent activation in the MTLs. [83],[84] By combining language with fMRI and voxel-based morphometry in patients with left-sided mesial TLE and HS, [85] studied whether atypical language dominance is associated with temporal and/or extra-temporal cortical volume changes. They found that patients with atypical language lateralization had more volumes of grey matter, mainly within right-sided temporolateral cortex, and less significantly within frontal brain regions compared to patients with typical language lateralization. Patients with atypical language lateralization did not differ in terms of language performance from patients with typical language dominanc,e. Atypical language lateralization in patients with left-sided mesial TLE was also associated with increased grey matter volume within the nonepileptic right temporal and frontal lobe. The exact meaning of these findings in language-related impairments of TLE patients is not clear and requires further elaborate studies for clarification.

An important research, one of its kind was conducted by, [86] for word recognition in patients with long-standing, medically-intractable epilepsy localized to the left or right temporal lobe. Participants were asked to read words that varied in the frequency of their spelling-to-sound correspondences. For the right temporal lobe group, reaction times (RTs) showed the same pattern across spelling-to-sound correspondence conditions as previously reported for normal participants. For the left temporal lobe group, however, the pattern of RTs suggested that performance was worse on words whose orthographic body was less frequent in the language.

Other cognitive impairments

As the evidence of various cognitive and language related impairments in TLE became stronger, the scientific committee's interest toward exploring the intactness of other cognitive domains also began to rise. Emotional cognition is one such domain, which includes sub-domains like facial recognition and theory of mind (ToM). A detailed description of all these cognitive aspects is out of scope of this review so we just make passing remarks on them. The amygdala has been implicated in the recognition of facial emotions, especially fearful expressions. Golouboff [87] investigated the recognition of facial emotions in children and adolescents, 8-16-years-old, in patients suffering from both TLE and ECE. Each was matched on age and gender with a control subject. Subjects were asked to label the emotions expressed in pictures of children's faces miming five basic emotions (happiness, sadness, fear, disgust, and anger) or neutrality (no emotion). All groups of children with epilepsy performed less well than controls. Authors also found that early seizure onset was associated with poor recognition of facial expression of emotion in TLE group, particularly for fear. Based on their findings, they concluded that early-onset TLE can compromise the development of recognizing facial expressions of emotion in children and adolescents and suggested a link between impaired fear recognition and behavioral disorders.

Another cognitive domain, although studied scarcely at the best is the ToM. ToM is a crucial aspect of social cognition and is mediated by a complex neural network. Studies on TLE suggest that its neuropathological involvement is not limited to temporal lobes as such and instead involves several other brain areas. Some of these regions seem to overlap the neural network responsible for ToM, and this overlap could possibly exist in TLE. Studies on whether TLE patients evidence ToM deficit, however, are scant and controversial. Li et al. [88] examined whether ToM deficit is evident in TLE. Thirty-one TLE patients and 24 matched controls were recruited and completed four tasks measuring different levels of ToM: False belief, faux pas recognition, processing of implied meanings, and cartoon ToM. The patients were impaired in both basic and advanced ToM. Right TLE had a more wide-ranging picture of deficit than left TLE. ToM appears to be vulnerable to TLE, especially on the right side. Since ToM might contribute to patients' psychosocial adjustment, a ToM measure should be included in regular neuropsychological assessments of such patients.

Other forms of social cognitive functions have also been studied. Social cognitive function refers collectively to the higher cognitive functions that are essential in our social lives, and its representative aspects are facial expression recognition and decision-making. Yamano et al. [89] conducted a study on the social cognitive function (decision-making) of patients with TLE, and found that this function is impaired, and that the right amygdalohippocampal complexes play an important role. Schacher et al. [54] investigated advanced ToM capacity in TLE through the detection of social faux pas, a sensitive indicator of higher-order deficits. The task requires the detection of social faux-pas (i.e., where someone makes a social blunder) from short scenarios. The performance of MTLE patients was compared to demographically matched nonmedial TLE patients and healthy controls. The MTLE group performed less well than both nonmedial TLE patients and healthy controls in detecting social faux pas. There was no significant difference in performance between nonmedial TLE and controls. Across both epilepsy groups, performance did not correlate with epilepsy related variables such as age at seizure onset, or duration of epilepsy. The authors suggested that MTL damage particularly that involving the amygdale was the specific cause of the deficit. [54]


   Conclusion Top


Temporal lobe epilepsy affects several domains of cognitive functioning. A plethora of studies have established dysfunctions in the domains of memory, executive functioning, language, emotional cognition, and social cognition. However, other domains are affected as well. Neurobiological basis of all these impairments remain to be known and understood. In future, several of such studies are needed whose results will have enormous implications in treatment as well as in prognosis of such patients.

 
   References Top

1.
Hermann BP, Seidenberg M, Dow C, Jones J, Rutecki P, Bhattacharya A, et al. Cognitive prognosis in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Ann Neurol 2006;60:80-7.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.
Hermann B, Seidenberg M, Lee EJ, Chan F, Rutecki P. Cognitive phenotypes in temporal lobe epilepsy. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007;13:12-20.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Keary TA, Frazier TW, Busch RM, Kubu CS, Iampietro M. Multivariate neuropsychological prediction of seizure lateralization in temporal epilepsy surgical cases. Epilepsia 2007;48:1438-46.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.
Zeman A, Jones-Gotman M, Kapur N. Epilepsy and Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Baddeley A. The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci 2000;4:417-23.  Back to cited text no. 5
[PUBMED]    
6.
Abrahams S, Morris RG, Polkey CE, Jarosz JM, Cox TC, Graves M, et al. Hippocampal involvement in spatial and working memory: A structural MRI analysis of patients with unilateral mesial temporal lobe sclerosis. Brain Cogn 1999;41:39-65.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
Black LC, Schefft BK, Howe SR, Szaflarski JP, Yeh HS, Privitera MD. The effect of seizures on working memory and executive functioning performance. Epilepsy Behav 2010;17:412-9.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Owen AM, Morris RG, Sahakian BJ, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Double dissociations of memory and executive functions in working memory tasks following frontal lobe excisions, temporal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Brain 1996;119:1597-615.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Wagner DD, Sziklas V, Garver KE, Jones-Gotman M. Material-specific lateralization of working memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:112-22.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
Hötting K, Katz-Biletzky T, Malina T, Lindenau M, Bengner T. Long-term versus short-term memory deficits for faces in temporal lobe and generalized epilepsy patients. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:574-8.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.
Howard CE, Andrés P, Broks P, Noad R, Sadler M, Coker D, et al. Memory, metamemory and their dissociation in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia 2010;48:921-32.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
Düzel E, Hufnagel A, Helmstaedter C, Elger C. Verbal working memory components can be selectively influenced by transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia 1996;34:775-83.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.
Campo P, Garrido MI, Moran RJ, Maestú F, García-Morales I, Gil-Nagel A, et al. Remote effects of hippocampal sclerosis on effective connectivity during working memory encoding: A case of connectional diaschisis? Cereb Cortex 2012;22:1225-36.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.
Cowey CM, Green S. The hippocampus: A "working memory" structure? The effect of hippocampal sclerosis on working memory. Memory 1996;4:19-30.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Grippo A, Pelosi L, Mehta V, Blumhardt LD. Working memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: An event-related potential study. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1996;99:200-13.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Krauss GL, Summerfield M, Brandt J, Breiter S, Ruchkin D. Mesial temporal spikes interfere with working memory. Neurology 1997;49:975-80.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.
Axmacher N, Mormann F, Fernández G, Cohen MX, Elger CE, Fell J. Sustained neural activity patterns during working memory in the human medial temporal lobe. J Neurosci 2007;27:7807-16.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Axmacher N, Schmitz DP, Wagner T, Elger CE, Fell J. Interactions between medial temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, and inferior temporal regions during visual working memory: A combined intracranial EEG and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 2008;28:7304-12.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Axmacher N, Elger CE, Fell J. Working memory-related hippocampal deactivation interferes with long-term memory formation. J Neurosci 2009;29:1052-960.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.
Campo P, Maestú F, García-Morales I, Gil-Nagel A, Strange B, Morales M, et al. Modulation of medial temporal lobe activity in epilepsy patients with hippocampal sclerosis during verbal working memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:536-46.  Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.
Cashdollar N, Malecki U, Rugg-Gunn FJ, Duncan JS, Lavie N, Duzel E. Hippocampus-dependent and -independent theta-networks of active maintenance. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009;106:20493-8.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.
Vlooswijk MC, Jansen JF, Jeukens CR, Majoie HJ, Hofman PA, de Krom MC, et al. Memory processes and prefrontal network dysfunction in cryptogenic epilepsy. Epilepsia 2011;52:1467-75.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.
Kapur N, Millar J, Colbourn C, Abbott P, Kennedy P, Docherty T. Very long-term amnesia in association with temporal lobe epilepsy: Evidence for multiple-stage consolidation processes. Brain Cogn 1997;35:58-70.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.
Blake RV, Wroe SJ, Breen EK, McCarthy RA. Accelerated forgetting in patients with epilepsy: Evidence for an impairment in memory consolidation. Brain 2000;123 Pt 3:472-83.  Back to cited text no. 24
    
25.
Butler CR, Zeman AZ. Recent insights into the impairment of memory in epilepsy: Transient epileptic amnesia, accelerated long-term forgetting and remote memory impairment. Brain 2008;131:2243-63.  Back to cited text no. 25
    
26.
Kapur N. Transient epileptic amnesia - A clinical update and a reformulation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1993;56:1184-90.  Back to cited text no. 26
[PUBMED]    
27.
Zeman AZ, Boniface SJ, Hodges JR. Transient epileptic amnesia: A description of the clinical and neuropsychological features in 10 cases and a review of the literature. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;64:435-43.  Back to cited text no. 27
    
28.
Manes F, Graham KS, Zeman A, de Luján Calcagno M, Hodges JR. Autobiographical amnesia and accelerated forgetting in transient epileptic amnesia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76:1387-91.  Back to cited text no. 28
    
29.
Butler CR, Graham KS, Hodges JR, Kapur N, Wardlaw JM, Zeman AZ. The syndrome of transient epileptic amnesia. Ann Neurol 2007;61:587-98.  Back to cited text no. 29
    
30.
Milton F, Muhlert N, Pindus DM, Butler CR, Kapur N, Graham KS, et al. Remote memory deficits in transient epileptic amnesia. Brain 2010;133:1368-79.  Back to cited text no. 30
    
31.
Illman NA, Rathbone CJ, Kemp S, Moulin CJ. Autobiographical memory and the self in a case of transient epileptic amnesia. Epilepsy Behav 2011;21:36-41.  Back to cited text no. 31
    
32.
Ioannidis P, Balamoutsos G, Karabela O, Kosmidis MH, Karacostas D. Transient epileptic amnesia in a memory clinic setting: A report of three cases. Epilepsy Behav 2011;20:414-7.  Back to cited text no. 32
    
33.
Kopelman MD, Wilson BA, Baddeley AD. The autobiographical memory interview: A new assessment of autobiographical and personal semantic memory in amnesic patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1989;11:724-44.  Back to cited text no. 33
    
34.
Manes F, Hodges JR, Graham KS, Zeman A. Focal autobiographical amnesia in association with transient epileptic amnesia. Brain 2001;124:499-509.  Back to cited text no. 34
    
35.
Milton F, Butler CR, Zeman AZ. Transient epileptic amnesia: Déjà vu heralding recovery of lost memories. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011;82:1178-9.  Back to cited text no. 35
[PUBMED]    
36.
Tramoni E, Felician O, Barbeau EJ, Guedj E, Guye M, Bartolomei F, et al. Long-term consolidation of declarative memory: Insight from temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 2011;134:816-31.  Back to cited text no. 36
    
37.
Hermann BP, Seidenberg M, Haltiner A, Wyler AR. Mood state in unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy. Biol Psychiatry 1991;30:1205-18.  Back to cited text no. 37
    
38.
Strauss E, Hunter M, Wada J. Wisconsin Card Sorting Performance: Effects of age of onset of damage and laterality of dysfunction. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1993;15:896-902.  Back to cited text no. 38
    
39.
Corcoran R, Upton D. A role for the hippocampus in card sorting? Cortex 1993;29:293-304.  Back to cited text no. 39
    
40.
Giovagnoli AR. Relation of sorting impairment to hippocampal damage in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia 2001;39:140-50.  Back to cited text no. 40
[PUBMED]    
41.
Drake M, Allegri RF, Thomson A. Executive cognitive alteration of prefrontal type in patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Medicina (B Aires) 2000;60:453-6.  Back to cited text no. 41
    
42.
Horner MD, Flashman LA, Freides D, Epstein CM, Bakay RA. Temporal lobe epilepsy and performance on the Wisconsin card sorting test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1996;18:310-3.  Back to cited text no. 42
    
43.
Kim CH, Lee SA, Yoo HJ, Kang JK, Lee JK. Executive performance on the Wisconsin card sorting test in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Eur Neurol 2007;57:39-46.  Back to cited text no. 43
    
44.
Oyegbile TO, Dow C, Jones J, Bell B, Rutecki P, Sheth R, et al. The nature and course of neuropsychological morbidity in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 2004;62:1736-42.  Back to cited text no. 44
    
45.
Allegri RF, Drake M, Thomson A. Neuropsychological findings in patients with middle temporal lobe epilepsy. Rev Neurol 1999;29:1160-3.  Back to cited text no. 45
    
46.
Garcia Espinosa A, Andrade Machado R, Borges González S, García González ME, Pérez Montoto A, Toledo Sotomayor G. Wisconsin card sorting test performance and impulsivity in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: Suicidal risk and suicide attempts. Epilepsy Behav 2010;17:39-45.  Back to cited text no. 46
    
47.
Rzezak P, Fuentes D, Guimarães CA, Thome-Souza S, Kuczynski E, Guerreiro M, et al. Executive dysfunction in children and adolescents with temporal lobe epilepsy: Is the Wisconsin card sorting test enough? Epilepsy Behav 2009;15:376-81.  Back to cited text no. 47
    
48.
Labudda K, Frigge K, Horstmann S, Aengenendt J, Woermann FG, Ebner A, et al. Decision making in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:50-8.  Back to cited text no. 48
    
49.
Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 1994;50:7-15.  Back to cited text no. 49
    
50.
McDonald CR, Delis DC, Norman MA, Tecoma ES, Iragui VJ. Discriminating patients with frontal-lobe epilepsy and temporal-lobe epilepsy: Utility of a multilevel design fluency test. Neuropsychology 2005;19:806-13.  Back to cited text no. 50
    
51.
Thompson PJ, Duncan JS. Cognitive decline in severe intractable epilepsy. Epilepsia 2005;46:1780-7.  Back to cited text no. 51
    
52.
Jokeit H, Seitz RJ, Markowitsch HJ, Neumann N, Witte OW, Ebner A. Prefrontal asymmetric interictal glucose hypometabolism and cognitive impairment in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 1997;120:2283-94.  Back to cited text no. 52
    
53.
Oddo S, Solís P, Consalvo D, Giagante B, Silva W, D′Alessio L, et al. Mesial temporal lobe epilepsy and hippocampal sclerosis: Cognitive function assessment in Hispanic patients. Epilepsy Behav 2003;4:717-22.  Back to cited text no. 53
    
54.
Schacher M, Winkler R, Grunwald T, Kraemer G, Kurthen M, Reed V, et al. Mesial temporal lobe epilepsy impairs advanced social cognition. Epilepsia 2006;47:2141-6.  Back to cited text no. 54
    
55.
Takaya S, Hanakawa T, Hashikawa K, Ikeda A, Sawamoto N, Nagamine T, et al. Prefrontal hypofunction in patients with intractable mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 2006;67:1674-6.  Back to cited text no. 55
    
56.
Wang XQ, Iang SY, Lu H, Ma L, Mao YL, Yang F. [Executive function impairment in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: Neuropsychological and diffusion-tensor imaging study]. Zhonghua Yi Xue Za Zhi 2007;87:3183-7.  Back to cited text no. 56
    
57.
Cahn-Weiner DA, Wittenberg D, McDonald C. Everyday cognition in temporal lobe and frontal lobe epilepsy. Epileptic Disord 2009;11:222-7.  Back to cited text no. 57
    
58.
Keller SS, Baker G, Downes JJ, Roberts N. Quantitative MRI of the prefrontal cortex and executive function in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 2009;15:186-95.  Back to cited text no. 58
    
59.
Bell BD, Seidenberg M, Hermann BP, Douville K. Visual and auditory naming in patients with left or bilateral temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 2003;55:29-37.  Back to cited text no. 59
    
60.
Loring DW, Meador KJ, Lee GP. Effects of temporal lobectomy on generative fluency and other language functions. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1994;9:229-38.  Back to cited text no. 60
    
61.
Mayeux R, Brandt J, Rosen J, Benson DF. Interictal memory and language impairment in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 1980;30:120-5.  Back to cited text no. 61
[PUBMED]    
62.
Lomlomdjian C, Solis P, Medel N, Kochen S. A study of word finding difficulties in Spanish speakers with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 2011;97:37-44.  Back to cited text no. 62
    
63.
Hamberger MJ, Tamny TR. Auditory naming and temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 1999;35:229-43.  Back to cited text no. 63
    
64.
Hamberger MJ, Seidel WT. Auditory and visual naming tests: Normative and patient data for accuracy, response time, and tip-of-the-tongue. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2003;9:479-89.  Back to cited text no. 64
    
65.
Hirsch J, Moreno DR, Kim KH. Interconnected large-scale systems for three fundamental cognitive tasks revealed by functional MRI. J Cogn Neurosci 2001;13:389-405.  Back to cited text no. 65
    
66.
Abrahams S, Goldstein LH, Simmons A, Brammer MJ, Williams SC, Giampietro VP, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of verbal fluency and confrontation naming using compressed image acquisition to permit overt responses. Hum Brain Mapp 2003;20:29-40.  Back to cited text no. 66
    
67.
Grabowski TJ, Damasio H, Eichhorn GR, Tranel D. Effects of gender on blood flow correlates of naming concrete entities. Neuroimage 2003;20:940-54.  Back to cited text no. 67
    
68.
Indefrey P, Levelt WJ. The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition 2004;92:101-44.  Back to cited text no. 68
    
69.
Price CJ, Devlin JT, Moore CJ, Morton C, Laird AR. Meta-analyses of object naming: Effect of baseline. Hum Brain Mapp 2005;25:70-82.  Back to cited text no. 69
    
70.
Kemeny S, Xu J, Park GH, Hosey LA, Wettig CM, Braun AR. Temporal dissociation of early lexical access and articulation using a delayed naming task - An FMRI study. Cereb Cortex 2006;16:587-95.  Back to cited text no. 70
    
71.
Saccuman MC, Cappa SF, Bates EA, Arevalo A, Della Rosa P, Danna M, et al. The impact of semantic reference on word class: An fMRI study of action and object naming. Neuroimage 2006;32:1865-78.  Back to cited text no. 71
    
72.
Hamberger MJ, McClelland S 3 rd , McKhann GM 2 nd , Williams AC, Goodman RR. Distribution of auditory and visual naming sites in nonlesional temporal lobe epilepsy patients and patients with space-occupying temporal lobe lesions. Epilepsia 2007;48:531-8.  Back to cited text no. 72
    
73.
Damasio H, Tranel D, Grabowski T, Adolphs R, Damasio A. Neural systems behind word and concept retrieval. Cognition 2004;92:179-229.  Back to cited text no. 73
    
74.
Jensen EJ, Hargreaves IS, Pexman PM, Bass A, Goodyear BG, Federico P. Abnormalities of lexical and semantic processing in left temporal lobe epilepsy: An fMRI study. Epilepsia 2011;52:2013-21.  Back to cited text no. 74
    
75.
Devinsky O. The myth of silent cortex and the morbidity of epileptogenic tissue: Implications for temporal lobectomy. Epilepsy Behav 2005;7:383-9.  Back to cited text no. 75
[PUBMED]    
76.
Hamberger MJ, Seidel WT, Goodman RR, Perrine K, McKhann GM. Temporal lobe stimulation reveals anatomic distinction between auditory naming processes. Neurology 2003;60:1478-83.  Back to cited text no. 76
    
77.
Drane DL, Ojemann GA, Aylward E, Ojemann JG, Johnson LC, Silbergeld DL, et al. Category-specific naming and recognition deficits in temporal lobe epilepsy surgical patients. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:1242-55.  Back to cited text no. 77
    
78.
Trebuchon-Da Fonseca A, Guedj E, Alario FX, Laguitton V, Mundler O, Chauvel P, et al. Brain regions underlying word finding difficulties in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 2009;132:2772-84.  Back to cited text no. 78
    
79.
Hamberger MJ, Seidel WT, McKhann GM 2 nd , Perrine K, Goodman RR. Brain stimulation reveals critical auditory naming cortex. Brain 2005;128:2742-9.  Back to cited text no. 79
    
80.
Hara K, Ohta K, Miyajima M, Hara M, Iino H, Matsuda A, et al. Mismatch negativity for speech sounds in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 2012;23:335-41.  Back to cited text no. 80
    
81.
Rutten GJ, Ramsey NF, van Rijen PC, Noordmans HJ, van Veelen CW. Development of a functional magnetic resonance imaging protocol for intraoperative localization of critical temporoparietal language areas. Ann Neurol 2002;51:350-60.  Back to cited text no. 81
    
82.
Rutten GJ, Ramsey NF, van Rijen PC, Alpherts WC, van Veelen CW. FMRI-determined language lateralization in patients with unilateral or mixed language dominance according to the Wada test. Neuroimage 2002;17:447-60.  Back to cited text no. 82
    
83.
Friedman L, Kenny JT, Wise AL, Wu D, Stuve TA, Miller DA, et al. Brain activation during silent word generation evaluated with functional MRI. Brain Lang 1998;64:231-56.  Back to cited text no. 83
    
84.
Phelps EA, Hyder F, Blamire AM, Shulman RG. FMRI of the prefrontal cortex during overt verbal fluency. Neuroreport 1997;8:561-5.  Back to cited text no. 84
    
85.
Labudda K, Mertens M, Janszky J, Bien CG, Woermann FG. Atypical language lateralisation associated with right fronto-temporal grey matter increases - A combined fMRI and VBM study in left-sided mesial temporal lobe epilepsy patients. Neuroimage 2012;59:728-37.  Back to cited text no. 85
    
86.
Ledoux K, Gordon B. Disruption of spelling-to-sound correspondence mapping during single-word reading in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Lang 2011;118:1-8.  Back to cited text no. 86
    
87.
Golouboff N, Fiori N, Delalande O, Fohlen M, Dellatolas G, Jambaqué I. Impaired facial expression recognition in children with temporal lobe epilepsy: Impact of early seizure onset on fear recognition. Epilepsy Behav 2012;23:335-41.  Back to cited text no. 87
    
88.
Li YH, Chiu MJ, Yeh ZT, Liou HH, Cheng TW, Hua MS. Theory of mind in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2013;19:594-600.  Back to cited text no. 88
    
89.
Yamano M, Akamatsu N, Tsuji S. Advances in research on cognitive function related to temporal lobe epilepsy: Focus on social cognitive function. J UOEH 2012;34:245-58  Back to cited text no. 89
    


    Figures

  [Figure 1]
 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3]



 

Top
Print this article  Email this article

    

 
   Search
 
  
    Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
    Article in PDF (570 KB)
    Citation Manager
    Access Statistics
    Reader Comments
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  


    Abstract
   Introduction
   Conclusion
    References
    Article Figures
    Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed3025    
    Printed60    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded130    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal