Annals of Indian Academy of Neurology
  Users Online: 111 Home | About the Journal | InstructionsCurrent Issue | Back IssuesLogin      Print this page Email this page  Small font size Default font size Increase font size


 
Table of Contents
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Year : 2016  |  Volume : 19  |  Issue : 1  |  Page : 83-88
 

Study of visuospatial skill in patients with dementia


Department of Neurology, Bangur Institute of Neurosciences, Kolkata, West Bengal, India

Date of Submission10-May-2015
Date of Decision05-Jun-2015
Date of Acceptance21-Aug-2015
Date of Web Publication2-Feb-2016

Correspondence Address:
Atanu Biswas
Department of Neurology, Bangur Institute of Neurosciences, 52/1A, SN Pandit Street, Kolkata - 700 025, West Bengal
India
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/0972-2327.168636

Rights and Permissions

 

   Abstract 

Objectives: To assess the visuospatial function in different types of dementia with the visual object and space perception (VOSP) battery and to relate the degree of visuospatial dysfunction with different types and stages of dementia. Materials and Methods: A sample of 53 participants with dementia and equal number of age-, sex-, and education-matched controls were recruited for the study. Participants were evaluated for visuospatial skill using VOSP test battery. The scores of dementia patients were compared with controls and within dementia cohort scores were compared based on stage of dementia. Results: The dementia group scored low in all of the subtests of the VOSP battery in comparison to controls. Alzheimer's disease (AD), dementia of Lewy bodies (DLB), and vascular dementia (VaD) patients performed more poorly than controls in all subtests examining object perception and space perception. The three semantic variants of frontotemporal dementia (FTD) patients scored low in all four subtests of object perception, whereas behavioral variant FTD (bvFTD) patients performed normally. The scores deteriorated with the advancement of dementia in all patients from the dementia groups. Conclusions: Visuospatial function is significantly impaired in dementia patients particularly in AD, DLB, and VaD patients from the beginning, and the impairment is severe in advanced disease stages.


Keywords: Alzheimer′s disease (AD), clinical dementia rating (CDR), dementia of Lewy bodies (DLB), dementia, vascular dementia (VaD), visuospatial function, visual object and space perception (VOSP)


How to cite this article:
Pal A, Biswas A, Pandit A, Roy A, Guin D, Gangopadhyay G, Senapati AK. Study of visuospatial skill in patients with dementia. Ann Indian Acad Neurol 2016;19:83-8

How to cite this URL:
Pal A, Biswas A, Pandit A, Roy A, Guin D, Gangopadhyay G, Senapati AK. Study of visuospatial skill in patients with dementia. Ann Indian Acad Neurol [serial online] 2016 [cited 2017 Oct 19];19:83-8. Available from: http://www.annalsofian.org/text.asp?2016/19/1/83/168636



   Introduction Top


The visuospatial function involves identifying stimuli and their location in the environment. Studies indicate that the visuospatial tasks activate different cortical areas such as the Broadmann area V5, the superior parietal lobule, the parieto-occipital junction, and the premotor areas. [1],[2],[3] Visuospatial skills are the use of vision in the perception of objects in our environment and the spatial relationships between them. The pathways for processing visual information in the posterior cortex are segregated, such that the dorsal regions process space-based "where" information, and the ventral regions process object-based "what" information. The dorsal pathway projects rostrally via the superior longitudinal fasciculus from the dorsal occipital cortex to the posterior parietal cortex. The ventral pathway projects rostrally via the inferior longitudinal fasciculus from ventral occipital cortex into inferior temporal cortex.

Most of the commonly used neuropsychological tests assessing visual and spatial perception rely on additional cognitive abilities. Among these instruments is the visual object and space perception (VOSP) battery. This battery evaluates the space and the object perception and is based on the assumption that these perceptions are functionally independent. The subtests require very simple responses, each of which focuses on one component of visual perception while minimizing the involvement of other cognitive skills. The VOSP battery can be used to evaluate separately dorsal and ventral stream processing because it is divided into subtests that emphasize either object or space perception [4] Four visual object perception subtests evaluate the function and integrity of the ventral stream (what) from the ventral occipital cortex into the inferior temporal cortex and four visual space perception subtests evaluate functioning of the dorsal stream (where) from the dorsal occipital cortex to the posterior parietal cortex.

The VOSP battery seems to be sensitive to changes in visuospatial functioning in various diseases such as Alzheimer's disease (AD), posterior cortical atrophy (PCA), [5] dementia of Lewy bodies (DLB), [6] and vascular dementia (VaD). Additionally, VOSP has been reported to detect a lack of impairment in visuospatial functions in patients with Huntington's disease and atypical Parkinsonism.

In the present study, we aimed to evaluate visuospatial functioning with the VOSP battery in different types of dementia and to relate the degree of visuospatial dysfunction with different types and stages of dementia.


   Materials and Methods Top


Participants

We evaluated 53 patients with dementia. Participants were enrolled consecutively from the Cognitive Disorder Clinic of our institute from January 2012 to June 2013. Before conducting the study, permission was obtained from the Institutional Ethics Committee. A detailed history was gathered from participants and their family members followed by a clinical examination that included neurological examination and assessment of cognitive functions as per protocol. We used the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the initial screening of the patients. Further, we used the Kolkata Cognitive Test Battery [7] and we also separately tested the language, praxis performance, executive functioning, and visuospatial and visuoperceptual functioning using standard methods. Each patient was then subjected to investigations including hematological, biochemical, and radiological tests. A cranial magnetic resonance imaging or computerized tomography scan was also performed. Some special investigations (e.g. electroencephalogram and cerebrospinal fluid analysis) were undertaken wherever investigators thought they were necessary. Dementia was diagnosed using Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM IV) criteria [8] (1994) and staging was done as per the clinical dementia rating (CDR) scale. [9] We used the National Institute of Neurological and Communication Disorders/Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS-ADRDA) criteria [10] for diagnosis of AD, Neary criteria for diagnosis of behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) and semantic dementia, [11],[12] the Consensus criteria for diagnosis of DLB, [13] and NINDS-ARIEN criteria for diagnosis of VaD. [14] Healthy control participants were recruited from the healthy relatives of patients attending the outpatient departments of the hospital. Healthy participants had to report a normal neurological examination, a CDR score of 0, and an MMSE score equal to or greater than 28/30. All participants and, when applicable, their caregivers signed an institutional review board-approved research consent form before participating in the study. The illiterate participants and participants with visual impairment were excluded from both patient and control groups.

Instruments

Visuospatial skill was estimated using the VOSP test battery [4] that was composed of the following.

Screening test

The VOSP also contains a screening test that checks whether the participant's visual sensory capacities are sufficiently intact to permit further examination (Shape Detection Screening Test). The test consists of 20 stimuli: 10 of the stimuli contain an incomplete form of the letter "X," whereas the other 10 stimuli do not. The participant is required to determine whether an X is present. According to the test's manual, participants with a score of 15 or lower should not be further tested.

Visual object perception

Incomplete letters subtest

Within the objects perception part of the battery, the incomplete letters subtest consists of 20 letters that are 70% degraded. The participant has to identify the letters.

Silhouettes subtest

The subtest consists of 15 silhouettes of animals and 15 silhouettes of inanimate objects, drawn from an unusual perspective. Participants are required to identify the drawings.

Object decision subtest

For object decision, four figures are shown simultaneously to the participant and only one of these figures corresponds to a real object; the other three figures are nonsense form distracters. The participant is required to identify the real object, shown at a rotation of 75°.

Progressive silhouettes subtest

The progressive silhouettes subtest consists of two series of stimulus cards (depicting a gun and a trumpet), each one consisting of 10 silhouette drawings, with each successive drawing revealing progressively more details of the object. The participant is required to identify the object as early as possible (in this subtest, high scores imply poor performance).

Visual space perception

Dot counting subtest

Within the spatial perception part of the VOSP the first subtest is dot counting. This task consists of ten cards with five to nine dots on each card. The participant has to identify the number of dots presented on each stimulus card.

Position discrimination subtest

The position discrimination subtest consists of 20 cards. Each card shows two squares containing dots. The participant has to decide which square displays the dot in the center.

Number location subtest

The number location subtest consists of 10 cards. Each card shows two squares. The top square contains randomly placed numbers, whereas the lower square shows a black dot, placed in the same position as one of the numbers above. The participant has to identify the card in which the position of the dot matches with the position of the number.

Cube analysis subtest

The last subtest, cube analysis, presents 10 stimuli where the subject is asked to determine the number of cubes shown on each stimulus card.

Statistical analysis

Data analysis was performed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) 13.0 software. The demographic variables were analyzed from a descriptive viewpoint. Nonparametric tests were used to compare VOSP result variables and non-continuous variables (Mann-Whitney and Kruskal-Wallis test). The chosen significance level was 5% (P < .05).


   Results Top


The demographic and clinical data are given in [Table 1]. There was no difference between the dementia group and the control group with respect to gender, age, and education level. No difference was noted between various dementia subgroups and the controls.
Table 1: Demographic characteristics of patients of various types of dementia and comparison with control

Click here to view


Comparison of the scores of AD, VaD, FTD, and DLB patients with those of controls on the visuospatial evaluation (VOSP) is shown in [Table 2]. None of the participants failed on the screening test, which meant that all patients and controls were eligible to perform the other subtests.
Table 2: Comparison of performances of various dementia subgroups with control group

Click here to view


The AD patients (N = 31) showed significant impairment in all subtest of visuospatial function in comparison to the controls. Similarly, the performance scores of the VaD group (N = 11) was poor on all subtests in comparison to the controls, indicating significant impairment in object perception and space perception of visuospatial function.

Owing to the small size of the FTD (N = 8) and DLB (N = 3) groups, we could apply the Mann-Whitney U-test. In the FTD group, we found significant differences in performance scores in comparison to the controls in the shape detection screening test, the incomplete letters test, and the object decision test. Two of our semantic variant patients scored low in space perception (one in number location and one in cube analysis) and one of the bvFTD patients scored low in number location. Within the FTD group, the bvFTD participants performed well on these tests of object performance, semantic dementia (N = 3) patients performed poorly and the semantic dementia patients made the overall scores of FTD group low as compared to controls. The DLB patients also performed poorly as compared to controls in all subtests of object perception and space perception (P < .05).

[Table 3] shows that the mean performance scores on all of the VOSP subtests steadily declined with advancement of dementia in all subgroups. We performed a Kruskal-Wallis test to compare the performance of different dementia stages (CDR1, CDR2, CDR3) with respect to various VOSP subtests and found significant differences on all subtests and MMSEs. This result signifies that the performance worsens with the advancement of dementia. Comparing the performance of patients in mild (CDR1) versus moderate (CDR2) dementia and between mild (CDR1) and severe (CDR3) with the Mann-Whitney U-test, we found significant differences on all subtests. Although no differences were found for the moderate (CDR2) and severe (CDR3) groups, a difference was found for the MMSE scores across these groups.
Table 3: Comparison of performance of patients in different stages of dementia

Click here to view



   Discussion Top


The aim of the present study was to evaluate the visuospatial skill of the patients with different forms of dementia and in different stages of dementia. We selected age, sex, and education matched dementia and control groups to nullify their possible influence on VOSP performance. Akin to many previous studies, [15],[16] our AD patients performed poorly as compared to controls on all eight subtests (four object perception and four space perception) of VOSP.

AD can affect most aspects of visual processing with the impact on both dorsal and ventral stream areas. Patients are impaired in dorsal stream functions such as angle discrimination and motion perception, [17] and ventral stream functions such as the perceptual discrimination and recognition of faces, colors, and objects. [18] Contrast sensitivity deficits are also prominent in AD patients. [19] In AD patients, visuospatial functioning can be impaired in the beginning of the disease, and it gradually declines with deterioration of cognition over time. In two studies done by Quental et al. (2009, 2013) [15],[16] on visuospatial skill with early AD patients and controls, it was found that controls outperformed AD patients on all neuropsychological evaluations, indicating different types of impairments of visuospatial functioning in AD patients. The subtests of the VOSP battery were found to be sensitive for detecting this impairment in mild cases.

Our DLB subgroup also performed poorly in all VOSP subtests in comparison to controls. The DLB patients showed more severe and pervasive visuospatial, attentional, and executive impairments compared to the AD patients, whereas the AD patients showed more severe memory impairment compared to the DLB patients. Visuospatial deficits are an important component for differentiating DLB from AD. Mosimann et al. (2004) found that DLB and Parkinson's disease dementia (PDD) patients showed more severe impairments compared to the AD patients on tests tapping both ventral stream functioning (tests of object and form perception) and dorsal stream functioning (tests of dot position and motion perception). [20] In a study by Calderon et al. (2001), the DLB group showed impaired performance on fragmented letters subtest, object decision subtest, and cube analysis subtest relative to both control patients and patients with AD. [21]

Our VaD cohort also performed poorly on the VOSP subtests in comparison to controls, indicating significant impairment in object perception and space perception of visuospatial functioning. In a study by Clague et al. (2005), the VaD group showed impairment on tests of attention and executive functions, naming, object decision, and cube analysis from the VOSP. [22] Thus, patients of AD, VaD, and DLB showed significant derangement of visuospatial skill including both object perception and space perception.

Our FTD patient had impaired object perception. Out of the eight FTD patients, three were of the semantic variant and five were of the behavioral variant (bvFTD). Whereas all three patients of the semantic variant produced low scores on all four subtests of the object perception test, patients of bvFTD performed well on the tests of object perceptions. The persons with semantic dementia showed poor semantic knowledge and usually experienced difficulty in naming visually presented objects or their pictures. [23] Therefore, impaired performance of our patients on all object perception tests may be attributed to impairment of their semantic knowledge (anterior temporal lobe), rather than object perceptual problems (ventral stream of visual processing).

Two of our semantic variant patients scored low in space perception subtest (one in number location and one in cube analysis) and one bvFTD patient scored low in number location subtest. Impaired space perception is not seen in FTD patients and this observation is difficult to explain. This might be due to attentional problem of bvFTD patients. Consistent with the relative preservation of the posterior cortex, mild bvFTD patients usually perform normally on tests that assess "bottom-up" visual spatial perception, such as the subtests of the VOSP battery. [24],[25] In a study done by Rahaman et al. (1999) on eight patients with the frontal variant of FTD, all patients showed normal scores on all aspects of the VOSP tests, whereas other aspects of cognition were impaired. [25] In a study done by Snowden et al. (2004), 15 patients showing a semantic variant of FTD performed normally on subtests of the VOSP battery, with the exception of the silhouettes subtest, which requires recognition of pictures of animals and objects. [26]

The concept of visuospatial skill can be recast in terms of ventral (what) versus dorsal (where) visual processing streams. According to this widely accepted dichotomy, the ventral pathway, or stream of information, proceeds from the primary visual to the visual association areas in the inferior temporal region and underlies object recognition by the association of visual information with semantic knowledge about the perceived objects. Components of VOSP, which depend primarily on ventral processing, are, therefore, the object decision subtest, the silhouette identification test and the fragmented letters subtest. The dorsal pathway projects rostrally via the superior longitudinal fasciculus from the dorsal occipital cortex to the posterior parietal cortex. The dorsal stream is responsible for computing the location of objects in space, the guidance of hand movements during grasping and complex visuospatial analysis. The cube analysis subtest and other subtests of space perception of VOSP draw heavily on the dorsal pathway. Based on the current findings and knowledge, it seems that our patients with AD, DLB, and VaD had severe deficits in both dorsal and ventral processing streams. In our dementia cohort, the performance of visuospatial skill deteriorated as the disease advanced progressively from mild through severe stages, which indicates that visuospatial functioning progressively deteriorated with the progression of dementia.

Normal visuospatial skill is extremely important for everyday activities. It is required in every moment of our life. Life becomes difficult when visuospatial functioning is significantly impaired, as is the case of dementia patients. The issue, however, is often neglected by the treating physicians. Not many studies have been done so far on this subject. Early institution of specific rehabilitation therapy can help, if the disease is detected in an early phase of dementia. According to some recent research, visuospatial tests have tremendous potential as effective and sensitive biomarkers for dementia, particularly in AD patients. [27]

The current study was limited in a few ways. For example, we used clinically probable cases based on the standard criteria for diagnosing various dementia subtypes. The pathological diagnoses of our patients are not available. Thus, the results of the current study need to be interpreted with the possible fallacies of diagnostic accuracies of various clinical criteria. The low number of patients of FTD and DLB in our study was another limitation.

In summary, the present study supports the assumption that visuospatial tasks are valuable for diagnosing AD and other dementias like DLB, and VaD. Several subtests of the VOSP battery proved effective for detecting visuospatial impairment in AD and different forms of dementia. Further studies on a larger sample and correlating visuospatial function with other cognitive and neuropsychological domain are now needed to confirm our preliminary findings.

Financial support and sponsorship

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflicts of interest

All contributors receive salary from Government and nobody is getting any financial assistance from any commercial agencies. Thus, we declare that there is no competing interest involved in this study.

 
   References Top

1.
Katz B, Rimmer S. Ophthalmologic manifestations of Alzheimer's disease. Surv Ophthalmol 1989;34:31-43.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.
Strauss E, Sherman EM, Spreen O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms and Commentary. 3 rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Thiyagesh SN, Farrow TF, Parks RW, Accosta-Mesa H, Young C, Wilkinson ID, et al. The neural basis of visuospatial perception in Alzheimer's Neuroimaging 2009;172:109-16.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.
Warrington EK, James M. The Visual Object and Space Perception Battery. Bury St Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company; 1991.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Nestor PJ, Caine D, Fryer TD, Clarke J, Hodges JR. The topography of metabolic deficits in posterior cortical atrophy (the visual variant of Alzheimer's disease) with FDG-PET. J Neuro Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003;74:1521-9.   Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.
Krüger R, Kuhn W, Leenders KL, Sprengelmeyer R, Müller T, Woitalla D, et al. Familial Parkinsonism with synuclein pathology: Clinical and PET studies of A30P mutation carriers. Neurology 2001;56:1355-62.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
Das SK, Banerjee TK, Mukherjee CS, Bose P, Biswas A, Hazra A, et al. An urban community based study of cognitive functions among non-demented elderly population in India. Neurol Asia 2006;11:37-48.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV. 4 th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): Current version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993;43:2412-4.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology 1984;34:939-44.  Back to cited text no. 10
[PUBMED]    
11.
Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, Passant U, Stuss D, Black S, et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: A consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 1998;51:1546-54.   Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
Snowden J, Neary D, Mann D. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: Clinical and pathological relationships. Acta Neuropathol 2007;114:31-8.   Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.
McKeith IG, Galasko D, Kosaka K, Perry EK, Dickson DW, Hansen LA, et al. Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): Report of the consortium on DLB international workshop. Neurology 1996;47:1113-24.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.
Román GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, Cummings JL, Masdeu JC, Garcia JH, et al. Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 1993;43:250-60.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Quental NB, Brucki SM, Bueno OF. Visuospatial dysfunction in early Alzheimer's disease. Dement Neuropsychol 2009;3:234-40.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Quental NB, Brucki SM, Bueno OF. Visuospatial function in early Alzheimer's disease - the use of the Visual Object and Space Perception (VOSP) battery. PLoS One 2013;8:e68398.   Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.
Mapstone M, Dickerson K, Duffy CJ. Distinct mechanisms of impairment in cognitive ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Brain 2008;131:1618-29.   Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Cronin-Golomb A, Corkin S, Rizzo JF, Cohen J, Growdon JH, Banks KS. Visual dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease: Relation to normal aging. Ann Neurol 1991;29:41-52.   Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Gilmore GC, Groth KE, Thomas CW. Stimulus contrast and word reading speed in Alzheimer's disease. Exp Aging Res 2005;31:15-33.   Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.
Mosimann UP, Mather G, Wesnes KA, O'Brien JT, Burn DJ, McKeith IG. Visual perception in Parkinson disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 2004;63:2091-6.   Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.
Calderon J, Perry RJ, Erzinclioglu SW, Berrios GE, Dening TR, Hodges JR. Perception, attention, and working memory are disproportionately impaired in dementia with Lewy bodies compared with Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001;70:157-64.   Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.
Clague F, Dudas RB, Thompson SA, Graham KS, Hodges JR. Multidimensional measures of person knowledge and spatial associative learning: Can these be applied to the differentiation of Alzheimer's disease from frontotemporal and vascular dementia. Neuropsychologia 2005;43:1338-50.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.
Woollams AM, Cooper-Pye E, Hodges JR, Patterson K. Anomia: A doubly typical signature of semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:2503-14.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.
Perry RJ, Hodges JR. Differentiating frontal and temporal variant frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 2000;54:2277-84.   Back to cited text no. 24
    
25.
Rahman S, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, Rogers RD, Robbins TW. Specific cognitive deficit in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain 1999;122:1469-93.  Back to cited text no. 25
    
26.
Snowden JS, Thompson JC, Neary D. Knowledge of famous faces and names in semantic dementia. Brain 2004;127:860-72.  Back to cited text no. 26
    
27.
Mandal PK, Joshi J, Saharan S. Visuospatial perception: An emerging biomarker for Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis 2012;31(Suppl 3):S117-35.  Back to cited text no. 27
    



 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3]



 

Top
Print this article  Email this article

    

 
   Search
 
  
    Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
    Article in PDF (495 KB)
    Citation Manager
    Access Statistics
    Reader Comments
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  


    Abstract
   Introduction
    Materials and Me...
   Results
   Discussion
    References
    Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed1937    
    Printed41    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded56    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal